Journal articles
Williams AJ, Menneer T, Sidana M, Walker T, Maguire K, Mueller M, Paterson C, Leyshon M, Leyshon C, Seymour E, et al (In Press). Fostering Engagement with Health and Housing Innovation: Development of Participant Personas in a Social Housing Cohort (Preprint).
Abstract:
Fostering Engagement with Health and Housing Innovation: Development of Participant Personas in a Social Housing Cohort (Preprint)
. BACKGROUND
. Personas, based on customer or population data, are widely used to inform design decisions in the commercial sector. The variety of methods available means that personas can be produced from projects of different types and scale.
.
.
. OBJECTIVE
. This study aims to experiment with the use of personas that bring together data from a survey, household air measurements and electricity usage sensors, and an interview within a research and innovation project, with the aim of supporting eHealth and eWell-being product, process, and service development through broadening the engagement with and understanding of the data about the local community.
.
.
. METHODS
. The project participants were social housing residents (adults only) living in central Cornwall, a rural unitary authority in the United Kingdom. A total of 329 households were recruited between September 2017 and November 2018, with 235 (71.4%) providing complete baseline survey data on demographics, socioeconomic position, household composition, home environment, technology ownership, pet ownership, smoking, social cohesion, volunteering, caring, mental well-being, physical and mental health–related quality of life, and activity. K-prototype cluster analysis was used to identify 8 clusters among the baseline survey responses. The sensor and interview data were subsequently analyzed by cluster and the insights from all 3 data sources were brought together to produce the personas, known as the Smartline Archetypes.
.
.
. RESULTS
. The Smartline Archetypes proved to be an engaging way of presenting data, accessible to a broader group of stakeholders than those who accessed the raw anonymized data, thereby providing a vehicle for greater research engagement, innovation, and impact.
.
.
. CONCLUSIONS
. Through the adoption of a tool widely used in practice, research projects could generate greater policy and practical impact, while also becoming more transparent and open to the public.
.
Abstract.
Menneer T, Mueller M, Townley S (2023). A cluster analysis approach to sampling domestic properties for sensor deployment. Building and Environment, 231, 110032-110032.
Johnes C, Sharpe RA, Menneer T, Taylor T, Nestel P (2023). Using Sensor Data to Identify Factors Affecting Internal Air Quality within 279 Lower Income Households in Cornwall, South West of England.
International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health,
20(2), 1075-1075.
Abstract:
Using Sensor Data to Identify Factors Affecting Internal Air Quality within 279 Lower Income Households in Cornwall, South West of England
(1) Background: Poor air quality affects health and causes premature death and disease. Outdoor air quality has received significant attention, but there has been less focus on indoor air quality and what drives levels of diverse pollutants in the home, such as particulate matter, and the impact this has on health; (2) Methods: This study conducts analysis of cross-sectional data from the Smartline project. Analyses of data from 279 social housing properties with indoor sensor data were used to assess multiple factors that could impact levels of particulate matter. T-Tests and Anova tests were used to explore associations between elevated PM2.5 and building, household and smoking and vaping characteristics. Binary logistic regression was used to test the association between elevated particulate matter and self-reported health; (3) Results: of the multiple potential drivers of the particulate matter investigated, smoking and vaping were significantly associated with mean PM2.5. Following multivariate analysis, only smoking remained significantly associated with higher mean concentrations. Properties in which <15 cigarettes/day were smoked were predicted to have PM2.5 concentrations 9.06 µg/m3 higher (95% CI 6.4, 12.82, p ≤ 0.001) than those in which residents were non-smokers and 11.82 µg/m3 higher (95% CI 7.67, 18.19, p ≤ 0.001) where >15 cigarettes were smoked; (4) Conclusions: a total of 25% of social housing properties in this study experienced levels of indoor PM greater than WHO guideline levels for ambient air pollution. Although there are many factors that impact air quality, in this study the main driver was smoking. This highlights the importance of targeting smoking in indoor environments in future smoking cessation and control policy and practice and of understanding how pollutants interact in the home environment. There is also a need for further research into the impact on indoor air quality of vaping, particularly due to the rise in use and uncertainty of its long-term impact.
Abstract.
Zheng L, Mueller M, Luo C, Menneer T, Yan X (2023). Variations in whole-life carbon emissions of similar buildings in proximity: an analysis of 145 residential properties in Cornwall, UK. Energy and Buildings, 296, 113387-113387.
Menneer T, Qi Z, Taylor T, Paterson C, Tu G, Elliott LR, Morrissey K, Mueller M (2021). Changes in Domestic Energy and Water Usage during the UK COVID-19 Lockdown Using High-Resolution Temporal Data.
International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health,
18(13), 6818-6818.
Abstract:
Changes in Domestic Energy and Water Usage during the UK COVID-19 Lockdown Using High-Resolution Temporal Data
In response to the COVID-19 outbreak, the UK Government provided public health advice to stay at home from 16 March 2020, followed by instruction to stay at home (full lockdown) from 24 March 2020. We use data with high temporal resolution from utility sensors installed in 280 homes across social housing in Cornwall, UK, to test for changes in domestic electricity, gas and water usage in response to government guidance. Gas usage increased by 20% following advice to stay at home, the week before full lockdown, although no difference was seen during full lockdown itself. During full lockdown, morning electricity usage shifted to later in the day, decreasing at 6 a.m. and increasing at midday. These changes in energy were echoed in water usage, with a 17% increase and a one-hour delay in peak morning usage. Changes were consistent with people getting up later, spending more time at home and washing more during full lockdown. Evidence for these changes was also observed in later lockdowns, but not between lockdowns. Our findings suggest more compliance with an enforced stay-at-home message than with advice. We discuss implications for socioeconomically disadvantaged households given the indication of inability to achieve increased energy needs during the pandemic.
Abstract.
Williams AJ, Menneer T, Sidana M, Walker T, Maguire K, Mueller M, Paterson C, Leyshon M, Leyshon C, Seymour E, et al (2021). Fostering Engagement with Health and Housing Innovation: Development of Participant Personas in a Social Housing Cohort.
JMIR Public Health and Surveillance,
7(2), e25037-e25037.
Abstract:
Fostering Engagement with Health and Housing Innovation: Development of Participant Personas in a Social Housing Cohort
BackgroundPersonas, based on customer or population data, are widely used to inform design decisions in the commercial sector. The variety of methods available means that personas can be produced from projects of different types and scale.ObjectiveThis study aims to experiment with the use of personas that bring together data from a survey, household air measurements and electricity usage sensors, and an interview within a research and innovation project, with the aim of supporting eHealth and eWell-being product, process, and service development through broadening the engagement with and understanding of the data about the local community.MethodsThe project participants were social housing residents (adults only) living in central Cornwall, a rural unitary authority in the United Kingdom. A total of 329 households were recruited between September 2017 and November 2018, with 235 (71.4%) providing complete baseline survey data on demographics, socioeconomic position, household composition, home environment, technology ownership, pet ownership, smoking, social cohesion, volunteering, caring, mental well-being, physical and mental health–related quality of life, and activity. K-prototype cluster analysis was used to identify 8 clusters among the baseline survey responses. The sensor and interview data were subsequently analyzed by cluster and the insights from all 3 data sources were brought together to produce the personas, known as the Smartline Archetypes.ResultsThe Smartline Archetypes proved to be an engaging way of presenting data, accessible to a broader group of stakeholders than those who accessed the raw anonymized data, thereby providing a vehicle for greater research engagement, innovation, and impact.ConclusionsThrough the adoption of a tool widely used in practice, research projects could generate greater policy and practical impact, while also becoming more transparent and open to the public.
Abstract.
Menneer T, Mueller M, Sharpe RA, Townley S (2021). Modelling mould growth in domestic environments using relative humidity and temperature.
Building and EnvironmentAbstract:
Modelling mould growth in domestic environments using relative humidity and temperature
Damp and high levels of relative humidity (RH), typically above 70-80%, are known to provide mould-favourable conditions. Exposure to indoor mould contamination has been associated with an increased risk of developing and/or exacerbating a range of allergic and non-allergic diseases. The VTT model is a mathematical model of indoor mould growth that was developed based on surface readings of RH and temperature on wood in a controlled laboratory chamber. The model provides a mould index based on the environmental readings. We test the generalisability of this laboratory-based model to less-controlled domestic environments across different values of model parameters. Mould indices were generated using objective measurements of RH and temperature in the air, taken from sensors in a domestic setting every 3-5 minutes over 1 year in the living room and bedroom across 219 homes. Mould indices were assessed against self-reports from occupants regarding the presence of visible mould growth and mouldy odour in the home. Logistic regression provided evidence for relationships between mould indices and occupant responses. Mould indices were most successful at predicting occupant responses when the model parameters encouraged higher vulnerability to mould growth compared with the original VTT model. A lower critical RH level, above which mould grows, a higher sensitivity, and larger increases in the mould index all consistently increased performance. Using moment-to-moment time-series data for temperature and RH, the model and its developments could help inform smart monitoring or control of RH, for example to counter risks associated with reduced ventilation in energy efficient homes.
Abstract.
Walker T, Menneer T, Leyshon C, Leyshon M, Williams AJ, Mueller M, Taylor T (2020). Determinants of Volunteering Within a Social Housing Community.
VOLUNTAS: International Journal of Voluntary and Nonprofit Organizations,
33(1), 188-200.
Abstract:
Determinants of Volunteering Within a Social Housing Community
AbstractIn general, research demonstrates that deprivation, education, health, and well-being are determinants of volunteering, and that volunteering can play an important role in building stronger communities and provides many benefits for individual health and well-being. This study concentrates on the effects of physical and mental health and well-being as predictors when the aspect of socio-economic impact has been minimised. It utilises a unique data set from a UK Housing Association community with generally high levels of deprivation. Data were analysed using bivariate probit regression. In contrast to previous findings, physical health and mental health were not significantly related to volunteering. The key finding was that mental well-being was significantly related to informal volunteering.
Abstract.
Godwin HJ, Menneer T, Liversedge SP, Cave KR, Holliman NS, Donnelly N (2020). Experience with searching in displays containing depth improves search performance by training participants to search more exhaustively. Acta Psychologica, 210, 103173-103173.
Menneer TSI, Cave KR, Kaplan E, Stroud MJ, Chang J, Donnelly N (2019). The relationship between working memory and the dual-target cost in visual search guidance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
Stroud MJ, Menneer TSI, Kaplan E, Cave KR, Donnelly N (2019). We can guide search by a set of colors, but are reluctant to do it. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics
Pugh S, Menneer T, Taunton D, Garner M, Donnelly N (2018). Exposure to acute psychosocial stress modulates the effect of cue validity in an attention orienting task. Journal of Vision, 18(10).
Hanson G, Taunton D, Menneer T, Donnelly N (2018). Fatigue causes lengthened giving-up times when the task is hard. Journal of Vision, 18(10).
Mestry N, Vos S, Menneer T, Donnelly N (2018). The Influence of Training with One or Two Faces on Dual-Target Face Search. Journal of Vision, 18(10).
Godwin HJ, Menneer T, Liversedge SP, Cave KR, Holliman NS, Donnelly N (2017). Adding depth to overlapping displays can improve visual search performance.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform,
43(8), 1532-1549.
Abstract:
Adding depth to overlapping displays can improve visual search performance.
Standard models of visual search have focused upon asking participants to search for a single target in displays where the objects do not overlap one another, and where the objects are presented on a single depth plane. This stands in contrast to many everyday visual searches wherein variations in overlap and depth are the norm, rather than the exception. Here, we addressed whether presenting overlapping objects on different depths planes to one another can improve search performance. Across 4 different experiments using different stimulus types (opaque polygons, transparent polygons, opaque real-world objects, and transparent X-ray images), we found that depth was primarily beneficial when the displays were transparent, and this benefit arose in terms of an increase in response accuracy. Although the benefit to search performance only appeared in some cases, across all stimulus types, we found evidence of marked shifts in eye-movement behavior. Our results have important implications for current models and theories of visual search, which have not yet provided detailed accounts of the effects that overlap and depth have on guidance and object identification processes. Moreover, our results show that the presence of depth information could aid real-world searches of complex, overlapping displays. (PsycINFO Database Record
Abstract.
Author URL.
Cave KR, Menneer T, Nomani MS, Stroud MJ, Donnelly N (2017). Dual Target Search is Neither Purely Simultaneous nor Purely Successive.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove), 1-29.
Abstract:
Dual Target Search is Neither Purely Simultaneous nor Purely Successive.
Previous research shows that visual search for two different targets is less efficient than search for a single target. Stroud, Menneer, Cave and Donnelly (2012) concluded that two target colours are represented separately based on modeling the fixation patterns. Although those analyses provide evidence for two separate target representations, they do not show whether participants search simultaneously for both targets, or first search for one target and then the other. Some studies suggest that multiple target representations are simultaneously active, while others indicate that search can be voluntarily simultaneous, or switching, or a mixture of both. Stroud et al.'s participants were not explicitly instructed to use any particular strategy. These data were revisited to determine which strategy was employed. Each fixated item was categorised according to whether its colour was more similar to one target or the other. Once an item similar to one target is fixated, the next fixated item is more likely to be similar to that target than the other, showing that at a given moment during search, one target is generally favoured. However, the search for one target is not completed before search for the other begins. Instead, there are often short runs of one or two fixations to distractors similar to one target, with each run followed by a switch to the other target. Thus, the results suggest that one target is more highly weighted than the other at any given time, but not to the extent that search is purely successive.
Abstract.
Author URL.
Mestry N, Menneer T, Cave KR, Godwin HJ, Donnelly N (2017). Dual-target cost in visual search for multiple unfamiliar faces.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform,
43(8), 1504-1519.
Abstract:
Dual-target cost in visual search for multiple unfamiliar faces.
The efficiency of visual search for one (single-target) and either of two (dual-target) unfamiliar faces was explored to understand the manifestations of capacity and guidance limitations in face search. The visual similarity of distractor faces to target faces was manipulated using morphing (Experiments 1 and 2) and multidimensional scaling (Experiment 3). A dual-target cost was found in all experiments, evidenced by slower and less accurate search in dual- than single-target conditions. The dual-target cost was unequal across the targets, with performance being maintained on one target and reduced on the other, which we label "preferred" and "non-preferred" respectively. We calculated the capacity for each target face and show reduced capacity for representing the non-preferred target face. However, results show that the capacity for the non-preferred target can be increased when the dual-target condition is conducted after participants complete the single-target conditions. Analyses of eye movements revealed evidence for weak guidance of fixations in single-target search, and when searching for the preferred target in dual-target search. Overall, the experiments show dual-target search for faces is capacity- and guidance-limited, leading to superior search for 1 face over the other in dual-target search. However, learning faces individually may improve capacity with the second face. (PsycINFO Database Record
Abstract.
Author URL.
Hanson G, Hillstrom A, Menneer T, Taunton D (2017). Effective task-switching behaviour despite fatigue by sleep restriction. Journal of Vision, 17(10).
Godwin HJ, Reichle ED, Menneer T (2017). Modeling Lag-2 Revisits to Understand Trade-Offs in Mixed Control of Fixation Termination During Visual Search.
Cogn Sci,
41(4), 996-1019.
Abstract:
Modeling Lag-2 Revisits to Understand Trade-Offs in Mixed Control of Fixation Termination During Visual Search.
An important question about eye-movement behavior is when the decision is made to terminate a fixation and program the following saccade. Different approaches have found converging evidence in favor of a mixed-control account, in which there is some overlap between processing information at fixation and planning the following saccade. We examined one interesting instance of mixed control in visual search: lag-2 revisits, during which observers fixate a stimulus, move to a different stimulus, and then revisit the first stimulus on the next fixation. Results show that the probability of lag-2 revisits occurring increased with the number of target-similar stimuli, and revisits were preceded by a brief fixation on the intervening distractor stimulus. We developed the Efficient Visual Sampling (EVS) computational model to simulate our findings (fixation durations and fixation locations) and to provide insight into mixed control of fixations and the perceptual, cognitive, and motor processes that produce lag-2 revisits.
Abstract.
Author URL.
Menneer T, Godwin HJ, Liversedge SP, Hillstrom AP, Benson V, Reichle ED, Donnelly N (2017). The FVF framework and target prevalence effects.
Behav Brain Sci,
40Abstract:
The FVF framework and target prevalence effects.
The Functional Visual Field (FVF) offers explanatory power. To us, it relates to existing literature on the flexibility of attentional focus in visual search and reading (Eriksen & St. James 1986; McConkie & Rayner 1975). The target article promotes reflection on existing findings. Here we consider the FVF as a mechanism in the Prevalence Effect (PE) in visual search.
Abstract.
Author URL.
Pugh S, Menneer T, Taunton D, Hillstrom A, Donnelly N (2017). The effects of acute stress on the attentional network. Journal of Vision, 17(10).
Glautier S, Menneer T, Godwin HJ, Donnelly N, Aristizabal JA (2016). Flexible Configural Learning of Non-Linear Discriminations and Detection of Stimulus Compounds.
Exp Psychol,
63(4), 215-236.
Abstract:
Flexible Configural Learning of Non-Linear Discriminations and Detection of Stimulus Compounds.
Previous work showed that prior experience with discriminations requiring configural solutions (e.g. biconditional discrimination) confers an advantage for the learning of new configural discriminations (e.g. negative patterning) in comparison to prior experience with elemental discriminations. This effect is well established but its mechanism is not well understood. In the studies described below we assessed whether the saliences of configural and element cues were affected by prior training. We observed positive transfer to a new configural discrimination after configural pre-training but we were unable to find evidence for changes in cue salience using a signal-detection task. Our results confirm previous work by demonstrating experience-dependent flexibility in cue processing but they also suggest that this flexibility occurs at a point in the stimulus processing pipeline later than 1-2 s after the presentation of stimulus inputs. (138 words).
Abstract.
Author URL.
Menneer T, Mestry N, Godwin H, Cave K, Donnelly N (2016). Measuring capacity for template precision in dual-target search for faces. Journal of Vision, 16(12).
Godwin H, Menneer T, Mestry N, Cave K, Donnelly N (2016). There and back again: Understanding the cause of revisits to distractors in high-prevalence visual searches. Journal of Vision, 16(12).
Godwin HJ, Menneer T, Riggs CA, Taunton D, Cave KR, Donnel N (2016). Understanding the contribution of target repetition and target expectation to the emergence of the prevalence effect in visual search.
Psychon Bull Rev,
23(3), 809-816.
Abstract:
Understanding the contribution of target repetition and target expectation to the emergence of the prevalence effect in visual search.
Behavior in visual search tasks is influenced by the proportion of trials on which a target is presented (the target prevalence). Previous research has shown that when target prevalence is low (2 % prevalence), participants tend to miss targets, as compared with higher prevalence levels (e.g. 50 % prevalence). There is an ongoing debate regarding the relative contributions of target repetition and the expectation that a target will occur in the emergence of prevalence effects. In order to disentangle these two factors, we went beyond previous studies by directly manipulating participants' expectations regarding how likely a target was to appear on a given trial. This we achieved without using cues or feedback. Our results indicated that both target repetition and target expectation contribute to the emergence of the prevalence effect.
Abstract.
Author URL.
Hout MC, Godwin HJ, Fitzsimmons G, Robbins A, Menneer T, Goldinger SD (2016). Using multidimensional scaling to quantify similarity in visual search and beyond.
Atten Percept Psychophys,
78(1), 3-20.
Abstract:
Using multidimensional scaling to quantify similarity in visual search and beyond.
Visual search is one of the most widely studied topics in vision science, both as an independent topic of interest, and as a tool for studying attention and visual cognition. A wide literature exists that seeks to understand how people find things under varying conditions of difficulty and complexity, and in situations ranging from the mundane (e.g. looking for one's keys) to those with significant societal importance (e.g. baggage or medical screening). A primary determinant of the ease and probability of success during search are the similarity relationships that exist in the search environment, such as the similarity between the background and the target, or the likeness of the non-targets to one another. A sense of similarity is often intuitive, but it is seldom quantified directly. This presents a problem in that similarity relationships are imprecisely specified, limiting the capacity of the researcher to examine adequately their influence. In this article, we present a novel approach to overcoming this problem that combines multi-dimensional scaling (MDS) analyses with behavioral and eye-tracking measurements. We propose a method whereby MDS can be repurposed to successfully quantify the similarity of experimental stimuli, thereby opening up theoretical questions in visual search and attention that cannot currently be addressed. These quantifications, in conjunction with behavioral and oculomotor measures, allow for critical observations about how similarity affects performance, information selection, and information processing. We provide a demonstration and tutorial of the approach, identify documented examples of its use, discuss how complementary computer vision methods could also be adopted, and close with a discussion of potential avenues for future application of this technique.
Abstract.
Author URL.
Mestry N, Menneer T, Godwin H, Cave K, Donnelly N (2015). Dual-Target Cost in Visual Search for Multiple Unfamiliar Faces. Journal of Vision, 15(12).
Menneer T, Cave K, Stroud M, Kaplan E, Donnelly N (2015). Modeling search guidance: Three parameters for characterizing performance in different types of visual search. Journal of Vision, 15(12).
Godwin HJ, Menneer T, Riggs CA, Cave KR, Donnelly N (2015). Perceptual failures in the selection and identification of low-prevalence targets in relative prevalence visual search.
Atten Percept Psychophys,
77(1), 150-159.
Abstract:
Perceptual failures in the selection and identification of low-prevalence targets in relative prevalence visual search.
Previous research has shown that during visual search tasks target prevalence (the proportion of trials in which a target appears) influences both the probability that a target will be detected, and the speed at which participants will quit searching and provide an 'absent' response. When prevalence is low (e.g. target presented on 2 % of trials), participants are less likely to detect the target than when prevalence is higher (e.g. 50 % of trials). In the present set of experiments, we examined perceptual failures to detect low prevalence targets in visual search. We used a relative prevalence search task in order to be able to present an overall 50 % target prevalence and thereby prevent the results being accounted for by early quitting behavior. Participants searched for two targets, one of which appeared on 45 % of trials and another that appeared on 5 % of trials, leaving overall target prevalence at 50 %. In the first experiment, participants searched for two dissimilar targets; in the second experiment, participants searched for two similar targets. Overall, the results supported the notion that a reduction in prevalence primarily influenced perceptual failures of identification, rather than of selection. Together, these experiments add to a growing body of research exploring how and why observers fail to detect low prevalence targets, especially in real-world tasks in which some targets are more likely to appear than others.
Abstract.
Author URL.
Godwin HJ, Menneer T, Cave KR, Thaibsyah M, Donnelly N (2015). The effects of increasing target prevalence on information processing during visual search.
Psychon Bull Rev,
22(2), 469-475.
Abstract:
The effects of increasing target prevalence on information processing during visual search.
The proportion of trials on which a target is presented (referred to as the target prevalence) during visual search influences the probability that the target will be detected. As prevalence increases, participants become biased toward reporting that the target is present. This bias results in an increase in detection rates for the target, coupled with an increased likelihood of making a false alarm. Previous work has demonstrated that, as prevalence increases, participants spend an increasing period of time searching on target-absent trials. The goal of the present study was to determine the information processing during the additional time spent searching on target-absent trials as prevalence increased. We recorded participants' eye movement behavior as they were engaged in low-prevalence (25% target-present trials), medium-prevalence (50%), or high-prevalence (75%) search. Increased prevalence primarily influenced search by increasing the time spent examining objects in the display, rather than by increasing the proportion of objects examined in each display. In addition, the additional time spent examining objects in high-prevalence target-absent trials was the result of revisiting objects. We discuss the implications of these results in relation to current models of search as well as ongoing efforts to alleviate the prevalence effect.
Abstract.
Author URL.
Mestry N, Menneer T, Wenger MJ, Benikos N, McCarthy RA, Donnelly N (2015). The role of configurality in the Thatcher illusion: an ERP study.
Psychon Bull Rev,
22(2), 445-452.
Abstract:
The role of configurality in the Thatcher illusion: an ERP study.
The Thatcher illusion (Thompson in Perception, 9, 483-484, 1980) is often explained as resulting from recognising a distortion of configural information when 'Thatcherised' faces are upright but not when inverted. However, recent behavioural studies suggest that there is an absence of perceptual configurality in upright Thatcherised faces (Donnelly et al. in Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 74, 1475-1487, 2012) and both perceptual and decisional sources of configurality in behavioural tasks with Thatcherised stimuli (Mestry, Menneer et al. in Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 456, 2012). To examine sources linked to the behavioural experience of the illusion, we studied inversion and Thatcherisation of faces (comparing across conditions in which no features, the eyes, the mouth, or both features were Thatcherised) on a set of event-related potential (ERP) components. Effects of inversion were found at the N170, P2 and P3b. Effects of eye condition were restricted to the N170 generated in the right hemisphere. Critically, an interaction of orientation and eye Thatcherisation was found for the P3b amplitude. Results from an individual with acquired prosopagnosia who can discriminate Thatcherised from typical faces but cannot categorise them or perceive the illusion (Mestry, Donnelly et al. in Neuropsychologia, 50, 3410-3418, 2012) only differed from typical participants at the P3b component. Findings suggest the P3b links most directly to the experience of the illusion. Overall, the study showed evidence consistent with both perceptual and decisional sources and the need to consider both in relation to configurality.
Abstract.
Author URL.
Godwin H, Menneer T, Liversedge S, Cave K, Holliman N, Donnelly N (2015). Visual Search for Transparent Overlapping Objects in Depth: Overlap Impairs Performance, but Depth does not benefit Performance. Journal of Vision, 15(12).
Mestry N, Menneer T, Wenger MJ, McCarthy R, Donnelly N (2014). A possible marker of configural processing at the N170: Converging evidence from typical participants and a case of prosopagnosia. Journal of Vision, 14(10), 1437-1437.
Godwin HJ, Reichle ED, Menneer T (2014). Coarse-to-fine eye movement behavior during visual search.
Psychon Bull Rev,
21(5), 1244-1249.
Abstract:
Coarse-to-fine eye movement behavior during visual search.
It has previously been argued that, during visual search, eye movement behavior is indicative of an underlying scanning "strategy" that starts on a global, or "coarse," scale but then progressively focuses to a more local, or "fine," scale. This conclusion is motivated by the finding that, as a trial progresses, fixation durations tend to increase and saccade amplitudes tend to decrease. In the present study, we replicate these effects but offer an alternative explanation for them-that they emerge from a few stochastic factors that control eye movement behavior. We report the results of a simulation supporting this hypothesis and discuss implications for future models of visual search.
Abstract.
Author URL.
Riggs CA, Godwin HJ, Menneer T, Liversedge SP, Donnelly N (2014). How does visual search behaviour adapt when partners have a response bias?. Journal of Vision, 14(10), 1205-1205.
Godwin HJ, Menneer T, Liversedge SP, Cave KR, Holliman NS, Donnelly N (2014). Searching for overlapping objects in depth: Depth speeds search, but does not improve response accuracy. Journal of Vision, 14(10), 1190-1190.
Menneer T, Godwin HJ, Hout MC (2014). Visual similarity is stronger than semantic similarity in guiding visual search for numbers. Journal of Vision, 14(10), 1201-1201.
Godwin HJ, Hout MC, Menneer T (2014). Visual similarity is stronger than semantic similarity in guiding visual search for numbers.
Psychon Bull Rev,
21(3), 689-695.
Abstract:
Visual similarity is stronger than semantic similarity in guiding visual search for numbers.
Using a visual search task, we explored how behavior is influenced by both visual and semantic information. We recorded participants' eye movements as they searched for a single target number in a search array of single-digit numbers (0-9). We examined the probability of fixating the various distractors as a function of two key dimensions: the visual similarity between the target and each distractor, and the semantic similarity (i.e. the numerical distance) between the target and each distractor. Visual similarity estimates were obtained using multidimensional scaling based on the independent observer similarity ratings. A linear mixed-effects model demonstrated that both visual and semantic similarity influenced the probability that distractors would be fixated. However, the visual similarity effect was substantially larger than the semantic similarity effect. We close by discussing the potential value of using this novel methodological approach and the implications for both simple and complex visual search displays.
Abstract.
Author URL.
Blaha L, Menneer T, Wenger M, Bittner JL (2013). Correlation Analysis for Multidimensional Signal Detection Evaluation and Comparison with Standard Analyses. Journal of Vision, 13(9), 1028-1028.
Menneer T, Wenger MJ, Blaha LM (2013). Probit analysis for multidimensional signal detection: an evaluation and comparison with standard analyses. Journal of Vision, 13(9), 107-107.
Mestry N, Wenger MJ, Menneer T, Benikos NP, Donnelly N (2013). The Impacts of Inversion and Thatcherisation on Face Processing: Mapping between ERP and GRT. Journal of Vision, 13(9), 391-391.
Godwin HJ, Menneer T, Donnelly N (2013). Weakened Target Representations in Low Prevalence Visual Search. Journal of Vision, 13(9), 528-528.
Donnelly N, Cornes K, Menneer T (2012). An examination of the processing capacity of features in the Thatcher illusion.
Atten Percept Psychophys,
74(7), 1475-1487.
Abstract:
An examination of the processing capacity of features in the Thatcher illusion.
Detection of the Thatcher illusion (Thompson, Perception, 9:483-484, 1980) is widely upheld as being dependent on configural processing (e.g. Bartlett & Searcy, Cognitive Psychology, 25:281-316, 1993; Boutsen, Humphreys, Praamstra, & Warbrick, NeuroImage, 32:352-367, 2006; Donnelly & Hadwin, Visual Cognition, 10:1001-1017, 2003; Leder & Bruce, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53A:513-536, 2000; Lewis, Perception, 30:769-774, 2001; Maurer, Grand, & Mondloch, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6:255-260, 2002; Stürzel & Spillmann, Perception, 29:937-942, 2000). Given that supercapacity processing accompanies configural processing (see Wenger & Townsend, 2001), supercapacity processing should occur in the processing of Thatcherised upright faces. The purpose of this study was to test for evidence that the grotesqueness of upright Thatcherised faces results from supercapacity processing. Two tasks were employed: categorisation of a single face as odd or normal, and a same/different task for sequentially presented faces. The stimuli were typical faces, partially Thatcherised faces (either eyes or mouth inverted) and fully Thatcherised faces. All of the faces were presented upright. The data from both experiments were analysed using mean response times and a number of capacity measures (capacity coefficient, the Miller and Grice inequalities, and the proportional-hazards ratio). The results of both experiments demonstrated some evidence of a redundancy gain for the redundant-target condition over the single-target condition, especially in the response times in Experiment 1. However, there was very limited evidence, in either experiment, that the redundancy gains resulted from supercapacity processing. We concluded that the oddity signalled by inversion of eyes and mouths does not arise from positive interdependencies between these features.
Abstract.
Author URL.
Mestry N, Donnelly N, Menneer T, McCarthy RA (2012). Discriminating Thatcherised from typical faces in a case of prosopagnosia.
Neuropsychologia,
50(14), 3410-3418.
Abstract:
Discriminating Thatcherised from typical faces in a case of prosopagnosia.
We report data from a prosopagnosic patient (PHD), and aged-matched control participants, from experiments where participants categorised individually presented emotional faces (experiment 1) and Thatcherised (from typical) faces (experiment 2). In experiment 2 participants also discriminated between simultaneously presented Thatcherised and typical faces. PHD was at chance categorising Thatcherised from typical faces. He was, however, able to discriminate between Thatcherised and typical faces, and partially able to categorise emotional faces. The results are discussed in terms of a loss of configural processing but preserved feature processing in PHD. The loss of configural processing impacts his categorisation of Thatcherised and typical faces, and his emotion processing, while his preserved feature processing supports his ability to categorise some emotional faces and his ability to discriminate between Thatcherised and typical faces.
Abstract.
Author URL.
Mestry N, Menneer T, Wenger MJ, Donnelly N (2012). Identifying sources of configurality in three face processing tasks.
Front Psychol,
3Abstract:
Identifying sources of configurality in three face processing tasks.
Participants performed three feature-complete face processing tasks involving detection of changes in: (1) feature size and (2) feature identity in successive matching tasks, and (3) feature orientation. In each experiment, information in the top (eyes) and bottom (mouths) parts of faces were manipulated. All tasks were performed with upright and inverted faces. Data were analyzed first using group-based analysis of signal detection measures (sensitivity and bias), and second using analysis of multidimensional measures of sensitivity and bias along with probit regression models in order to draw inferences about independence and separability as defined within general recognition theory (Ashby and Townsend, 1986). The results highlighted different patterns of perceptual and decisional influences across tasks and orientations. There was evidence of orientation specific configural effects (violations of perceptual independence, perceptual seperability and decisional separabilty) in the Feature Orientation Task. For the Feature Identity Task there were orientation specific performance effects and there was evidence of configural effects (violations of decisional separability) in both orientations. Decisional effects are consistent with previous research (Wenger and Ingvalson, 2002, 2003; Richler et al. 2008; Cornes et al. 2011). Crucially, the probit analysis revealed violations of perceptual independence that remain undetected by marginal analysis.
Abstract.
Author URL.
Menneer T, Stroud MJ, Cave KR, Li X, Godwin HJ, Liversedge SP, Donnelly N (2012). Search for two categories of target produces fewer fixations to target-color items.
J Exp Psychol Appl,
18(4), 404-418.
Abstract:
Search for two categories of target produces fewer fixations to target-color items.
Searching simultaneously for metal threats (guns and knives) and improvised explosive devices (IEDs) in X-ray images is less effective than 2 independent single-target searches, 1 for metal threats and 1 for IEDs. The goals of this study were to (a) replicate this dual-target cost for categorical targets and to determine whether the cost remains when X-ray images overlap, (b) determine the role of attentional guidance in this dual-target cost by measuring eye movements, and (c) determine the effect of practice on guidance. Untrained participants conducted 5,376 trials of visual search of X-ray images, each specializing in single-target search for metal threats, single-target search for IEDs, or dual-target search for both. In dual-target search, only 1 target (metal threat or IED) at most appeared on any 1 trial. Eye movements, response time, and accuracy were compared across single-target and dual-target searches. Results showed a dual-target cost in response time, accuracy, and guidance, with fewer fixations to target-color objects and disproportionately more to non-target-color objects, compared with single-target search. Such reduction in guidance explains why targets are missed in dual-target search, which was particularly noticeable when objects overlapped. After extensive practice, accuracy, response time, and guidance remained better in single-target search than in dual-target search. The results indicate that, when 2 different target representations are required for search, both representations cannot be maintained as accurately as in separate single-target searches. They suggest that baggage X-ray security screeners should specialize in one type of threat, or be trained to conduct 2 independent searches, 1 for each threat item.
Abstract.
Author URL.
Menneer T, Mestry N, Wenger M, Donnelly N (2012). Tests for Configural processing in the Thatcher Illusion. Journal of Vision, 12(9), 635-635.
Stroud MJ, Menneer T, Cave KR, Donnelly N (2012). Using the dual-target cost to explore the nature of search target representations.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform,
38(1), 113-122.
Abstract:
Using the dual-target cost to explore the nature of search target representations.
Eye movements were monitored to examine search efficiency and infer how color is mentally represented to guide search for multiple targets. Observers located a single color target very efficiently by fixating colors similar to the target. However, simultaneous search for 2 colors produced a dual-target cost. In addition, as the similarity between the 2 target colors decreased, search efficiency suffered, resulting in more fixations on colors dissimilar to both target colors, which we describe as a "split-target cost." the patterns of fixations provide evidence to the type of mental representations guiding search. When the 2 targets are dissimilar, they are apparently encoded as separate and discrete representations. The fixation patterns for more similar targets can be explained with either 2 discrete target representations or a single, unitary range containing the target colors as well as the colors between them in color space.
Abstract.
Author URL.
Stroud M, Kaplan E, Menneer T, Cave K, Donnelly N (2011). A Color in Working Memory Does Not Become a Search Target, but it Does Interfere with Color Search. Journal of Vision, 11(11), 1317-1317.
Stroud MJ, Menneer T, Cave KR, Donnelly N, Rayner K (2011). Search for multiple targets of different colours: Misguided eye movements reveal a reduction of colour selectivity.
APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY,
25(6), 971-982.
Author URL.
Mestry N, Menneer T, Godwin H, McCarthy R, Donnelly N (2010). Are deficits in emotional face processing preventing perception of the Thatcher illusion in a case of prosopagnosia?. Journal of Vision, 10(7), 588-588.
Menneer T, Silbert N, Cornes K, Wenger M, Townsend J, Donnelly N (2010). Contrasting methods of model estimation for configural and holistic perception. Journal of Vision, 9(8), 555-555.
Donnelly N, Menneer T, Cornes K, Mestry N, McCarthy R (2010). Detecting the Thatcher illusion in a case of prosopagnosia. Journal of Vision, 9(8), 557-557.
Godwin HJ, Menneer T, Cave KR, Helman S, Way RL, Donnelly N (2010). Don't distract the searcher: search performance for X-ray security screening images is reduced with the addition of a simple mental arithmetic task. Journal of Vision, 8(6), 319-319.
Godwin HJ, Menneer T, Cave KR, Donnelly N (2010). Dual-target search for high and low prevalence X-ray threat targets. Visual Cognition, 18(10), 1439-1463.
Menneer T, Donnelly N, Godwin HJ, Cave KR (2010). High or low target prevalence increases the dual-target cost in visual search.
J Exp Psychol Appl,
16(2), 133-144.
Abstract:
High or low target prevalence increases the dual-target cost in visual search.
Previous studies have demonstrated a dual-target cost in visual search. In the current study, the relationship between search for one and search for two targets was investigated to examine the effects of target prevalence and practice. Color-shape conjunction stimuli were used with response time, accuracy and signal detection measures. Performance was lower in dual-target search compared with the combined performance for two independent single-target searches. The cost in response time slope disappeared with practice, but the cost in accuracy remained. Sensitivity was lower and the decision criterion more conservative in dual-target search than in single-target searches, suggesting that the representation of the target was less effective in dual-target search than in single-target search. Manipulation of target prevalence induced a bias in favor of the more likely correct response: target-present responses were likely under high target prevalence and target-absent responses were likely under low target prevalence. The prevalence effect was greater in dual-target search than single-target search, causing the dual-target cost to be larger under target prevalences that differed from 50%. These findings are important for applied tasks in which targets appear rarely and can differ from each other. For example, the low target prevalence in X-ray security searches may magnify the dual-target cost implicated in previous research with X-ray images (see Menneer, Cave, & Donnelly, 2009). Such a result would increase the need for security personnel to consider alternatives to dual-target search, such as specialization in detecting one target type or training to encourage independent searches for each target.
Abstract.
Author URL.
Godwin HJ, Menneer T, Helman S, Cave KR, Donnelly N (2010). In difficult visual search, high frequency targets are found at the expense of low frequency targets. Journal of Vision, 7(9), 706-706.
Menneer T, Wenger M, Blaha L (2010). Inferential challenges for General Recognition Theory: Mean-shift Integrality and Perceptual Configurality. Journal of Vision, 10(7), 1211-1211.
Menneer T, Stroud MJ, Cave KR, Donnelly N (2010). Target representations guiding visual search for two colors: Two discrete colors, or a single range?. Journal of Vision, 9(8), 1182-1182.
Cave K, Menneer T, Stroud M, Donnelly N, Rayner K (2010). The breakdown of color selectivity in multitarget search: Evidence from Eye Movements. Journal of Vision, 7(9), 713-713.
Menneer T, Barrett DJ, Phillips L, Donnelly N, Cave KR (2010). The breakdown of efficient search when either of two colour targets can appear. Journal of Vision, 3(9), 568-568.
Menneer T, Li X, Stroud M, Butler C, Cave K, Nick D (2010). The effect of practice on top-down guidance in visual search for two types of complex target: Evidence from eye-movements. Journal of Vision, 8(6), 775-775.
Godwin HJ, Menneer T, Cave KR, Helman S, Way RL, Donnelly N (2010). The impact of Relative Prevalence on dual-target search for threat items from airport X-ray screening.
Acta Psychol (Amst),
134(1), 79-84.
Abstract:
The impact of Relative Prevalence on dual-target search for threat items from airport X-ray screening.
The probability of target presentation in visual search tasks influences target detection performance: this is known as the prevalence effect (Wolfe et al. 2005). Additionally, searching for several targets simultaneously reduces search performance: this is known as the dual-target cost (DTC: Menneer et al. 2007). The interaction between the DTC and prevalence effect was investigated in a single study by presenting one target in dual-target search at a higher level of prevalence than the other target (Target A: 45% Prevalence; Target B: 5% Prevalence). An overall DTC was found for both RTs and response accuracy. Furthermore, there was an effect of target prevalence in dual-target search, suggesting that, when one target is presented at a higher level of prevalence than the other, both the dual-target cost and the prevalence effect contribute to decrements in performance. The implications for airport X-ray screening are discussed.
Abstract.
Author URL.
Godwin HJ, Menneer T, Cave KR, Cutler V, Donnelly N (2010). The prevlanece effect is imbalanced: it is stronger for high target presentation rates, than for low. Journal of Vision, 9(8), 1171-1171.
Hillstrom AP, Menneer T, Donnelly N, Krokos M (2010). Using gaze measures to diagnose what guides search in complex displays. Journal of Vision, 8(6), 322-322.
Menneer T, Cave KR, Donnelly N (2009). The cost of search for multiple targets: effects of practice and target similarity.
J Exp Psychol Appl,
15(2), 125-139.
Abstract:
The cost of search for multiple targets: effects of practice and target similarity.
With the use of X-ray images, performance in the simultaneous search for two target categories was compared with performance in two independent searches, one for each category. In all cases, displays contained one target at most. Dual-target search, for both categories simultaneously, produced a cost in accuracy, although the magnitude of this dual-target cost was affected by the nature of the targets. When target feature sets shared values, accuracy in dual-target search was equivalent to that in the less accurate of the two single-target searches. However, when targets comprised different feature sets, accuracy in dual-target search was lower than in either single-target search. These results held after practice. In conclusion, dual-target search performance depends on the target representations required for search. When combined representations contain conflicting values within the most informative feature dimensions, then there is a cost in performance. When target representations share features, the search can be guided by the common values so that resources are not wasted on irrelevant distractors. The implication is that security screener performance might be improved by specializing in searching for threat categories that share features. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2009 APA, all rights reserved).
Abstract.
Author URL.
Welland M, Donnelly N, Menneer T (2006). Are we properly using our brains in seismic interpretation?. The Leading Edge, 25(2), 142-144.
Menneer T, Barrett DJK, Phillips L, Donnelly N, Cave KR (2006). Costs in searching for two targets: dividing search across target types could improve airport security screening.
Applied Cognitive Psychology,
21(7), 915-932.
Abstract:
Costs in searching for two targets: dividing search across target types could improve airport security screening
AbstractThe cost of searching for two visual targets simultaneously was compared against two separate single‐target searches using exposure time and accuracy measures within a staircase procedure. Dual‐target search for all stimuli (colour, shape and orientation) exhibited a loss of accuracy for one target. For orientation and shape, this dual‐target cost in accuracy was extreme, with chance‐level performance on one target. For colour, dual‐target search exhibited an additional cost in search time, with search requiring a longer exposure than the summed time required for two single‐target searches. An additional search‐time cost was also found for orientation targets when irrelevant colour variation was added to the display. In conclusion, dual‐target search for dissimilar targets is accompanied by an accuracy cost. Furthermore, colour variation, whether task‐relevant or not, leads to an additional cost in processing speed. The results suggest that a divided‐effort strategy would improve performance in search tasks such as X‐ray baggage screening. Copyright © 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Abstract.
Menneer T, Barrett DJ, Phillips L, Donnelly N, Cave KR (2004). The effect of training on search for complex stimuli. Journal of Vision, 4(8), 338-338.
Narayanan A, Menneer T (2000). Quantum artificial neural network architectures and components. Information Sciences, 128(3-4), 231-255.